Wednesday, November 30, 2005

Who do you want to be today?

Does it seem sometimes that your fantasy life is more important than your real one? That your virtual self is more real than your living, breathing body? Today I want to touch upon some aspects of psychology that are generally neglected by education assessment and measurement specialists.

Those worthies, though I'm sure they have childrens' best interests in mind, have learned to treat the world as something to be measured. In measuring it, they reduce its reality to a series of questions to be interrogated through hypothesis, measurement, analysis, and confirmation or disconfirmation. I'm talking about hardcore quantoid researchers, here, people. Please understand, I don't have a problem with statistically based research. I'm a big fan of using descriptive statistics to get a sense of the reality in which we live. However, a focus on the "empirical" can both obscure reality and neglect questions essential to the understanding of human cognition.

On the one hand, many educational researchers, especially those whose specialty is assessment, tend to try to make their jobs seem more simple than they actually are. When people assess student achievement, for example, they are primarily concerned about only two sources of error: reliability and validity. That is, can the same instrument produce the same results over time and it is actually measuring what it claims to measure. Noel Wilson (1998) suggests that there are at least thirteen sources of measurement error that can creep into such assessments (e.g., standardized tests, exit exams, etc.). So even there, researchers tend to assume a reality that is less chaotic, less messy, and, thus, more controllable than might actually be the case. Further, much of educational assessment tends to rely on a version of human psychology that pays a lot more attention to what we consciously think about and do than the darker, deeper, more occulted elements of the human psyche. Thus, it's important to think about fantasy and identification as two key elements of subjective life.

Think about it this way. Freud and Jung and Marx, among the various European philosophers who have located human motivation and consciousness outside of cognition, as such. While they have been critiqued by many, and for good reasons, we should not so easily dismiss a key element shared by their varied philosophies: We may not be completely in control. What we think and do may be based in part on volition and perception of empirical stimuli that guide that volition. It also, in many ways, is dictated by processes outside of our control or even awareness.

Freud, for example, introduced the notion of the unconscious, interpretation of dreams, and tried to explain how one's upbringing can result in conflicting elements within one's psyche, some of which is always outside of our knowledge and control. Certainly, he may have gotten a lot of things wrong, but at least he realized that we, as human beings, have a lot more going on in our brains that what we are aware of. Jung, similarly, tried to look at the bases of the human unconscious in symbolism and archetype. His approach, compared to Freud, was almost literary in its methods, relying heavily on the notions of narrative and symbolism. Both these theorists attempted to go beyond what experimental or behavioral psychology have attempted, rejecting the raw empiricist impulse, at least in part, in order to explore the deeper wildernesses of human thinking and motivation. Marx, in his own way, tried to do the same. In his case, he attempted to link individual consciousness with a kind of mass consciousness by applying poltical-economic elements to his understanding of society, its development, and its likely path. Note here, that Marx attempted to show the ways in which the material conditions of people's lives, and not just what was inside their heads, produced some elements of their consciousness of reality, whether they were aware of it or not.

My hero, Kenneth Burke, attempted to do this in a way that fused elements of Freud and Marx, among others. Let me add, here, the wikipedia article on Burke, as it makes as clear (or clearer) than I can, Burke's contributions:

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia.
Jump to: navigation, search

Kenneth Burke (1897 - 1993) was a major American literary theorist and philosopher. Burke's primary interests were in rhetoric and aesthetics.

Burke, like many twentieth century theorists and critics, was heavily influenced by the ideas of Karl Marx, Sigmund Freud, and Friedrich Nietzsche. He was also significantly influenced by Thorstein Veblen. Burke resisted being pigeonholed as a follower of any philosophical or political school of thought, and had a notable and very public break with the Marxists who dominated the literary criticism set in the 1930s. The political and social power of symbols was central to Burke's scholarship throughout his career. His political engagement is evident, for example, at the outset of A Grammar of Motives in its epigraph, ad bellum purificandum -- toward the purification of war, with "pure" war implying its elimination. Burke felt that the study of rhetoric would help human beings understand "what is involved when we say what people are doing and why they are doing it." Burke called such analysis "dramatism" and believed that such an approach to language analysis and use could help us understand the basis of conflict, the virtues and dangers of cooperation, and the opportunities of identification and consubstantiality.

Burke defined the rhetorical function of language as "the use of language as a symbolic means of inducing cooperation in beings that by nature respond to symbols." He defined "man" as "the symbol using, making, and mis-using animal, inventor of the negative, separated from his natural condition by instruments of his own making, and rotten with perfection." For Burke, some of the most significant problems in human behavior resulted from situations in which symbols used human beings rather than human beings using symbols.

In Burke's philosophy, social interaction and communication should be understood in terms of a pentad, which includes act, scene, agent, agency, and purpose. He proposed that most social interaction and communication can be approached as a form of drama whose outcomes are determined by ratios between these five pentadic elements. This has become known as the "dramatistic pentad." The pentad is grounded in his dramatistic method, which sees the relationship between life and theater as literal rather than metaphorical: for Burke, all the world really is a stage. Burke pursued literary criticism not as a formalistic enterprise but rather as an enterprise with significant sociological impact; he saw literature as "equipment for living," offering people folk wisdom and common sense to people and thus guiding the way people lived their lives.

Another key concept for Burke is the terministic screen -- a set of symbols that becomes a kind of screen or grid of intelligibility through which the world makes sense to us. Here Burke offers rhetorical theorists and critics a way of understanding the relationship between language and ideology. Language, Burke thought, doesn't simply "reflect" reality; it also helps select reality as well as deflect reality.

His principal works include:

* Counter-Statement (1931)
* Permanence and Change (1935)
* Attitudes Toward History (1937)
* Philosophy of Literary Form (1939)
* A Grammar of Motives (1945)
* A Rhetoric of Motives (1950)
* The Rhetoric of Religion (1961)
* Language as Symbolic Action (1966)



So Burke, similar to Jung, locates human consciousness in the human being, in the human being's symbol system(s), and in the human being's material and symbolic environment.

Of particular interest to me in all of these theorists of human consciousness is the idea that some things about human consciousness are quite hard to measure, if not impossible. More importantly, human motivation is incredibly complex, and not so simple to understand and control, even in the case of the young.

Sela (Pause and reflect)

What meanings, then, should we give to children's and teenagers' fantasy lives? How important are they? How important are their encounters with art, literature, and music? How does, as I suggested a few days ago, a lust for power and control result in the creation of a vivid and lived fantasy life? How does causing them to sit, learn, and obey virtually assure that they will find a place in their minds to go and be free. In some of my recent writing, I observed that:

... policy is one based not on humans as natural beings, with all that implies in terms of virtues and sins; instead of recognizing the elements of chaos and hope inherent in the enterprise of learning and living, [policy] views it much like a layman’s conception of a laboratory situation, where everything is known or knowable and everything under control. Real learning, like real science, is messier, and good policy, like good science, recognizes the potential for chaos, indeterminacy, and unexpected results.



One key element left out is the heart, the "gut music" as my friend Matt used to say, that drives us to do what we do. And where that comes from depends on the person. Freud, Marx, Jung and Burke each had an idea that it may not come from where we think it does. I agree.

1 comment:

Anonymous said...

I tend to believe in the value of measurement. However, it's certainly true that most educational and psychological assessments have substantial deficits in what they measure. And some things can't simply be quantified; they have to be explained verbally or visually.

I tend, however, to be suspicious of statistics, although it has substantial value at times. The thing is, statistics is about populations rather than individuals. An example where this causes blindness is in neuroimaging research. The vast majority of neuroimaging research involves dividing subjects into groups (i.e., by disorder, gender, trait, or age vs. controls). The groups are then compared, and trends are observed in the differences. However, this glosses over (I guess that term is semantically primed in my brain) the individual differences that exist, which are not so clear-cut, even if statistical comparisons of averages are possible. For a visual example, see http://www.uwm.edu/~neuropsy/fmri.html which shows 6 neuroimaging scans of subjects on a task. Each one is noticably different, and although groupable characteristics of the individuals (i.e., sex, age, disorder, trait) may influence or relate to their pattern, the whole combination of influences gives rise to activation patterns that may be as unique as a fingerprint (despite the same structures tending to be activated.)

Another example is neuroimaging research on psychopaths ("antisocial personality disorder"), in which accumulated research indicated that psychopaths, vs. controls, had patterns of abnormal brain strucutre and function. However, when psychopaths were subgrouped by whether they'd been arrested or not, it was found that only the ones who had been arrested had a much higher likelyhood of having this abnormality vs. controls. So it's an example of where some statistical tendency of a group is mediated by other aspects of the individuals involved: http://www.futurepundit.com/archives/001998.html

"When people assess student achievement, for example, they are primarily concerned about only two sources of error: reliability and validity"
The thing they are missing that I'm irate about is looking at things in enough detail. Like exams where I know tons on the subject, but the test didn't ask (because the class didn't cover it), but it's still worth measuring! :)

A cool thing about psycho-educational measurement, that I suspect many can relate to, can be observed by looking at video games and the benefits of feedback. Having feedback, when one is motivated to succed at or improve at a task, and the task isn't too easy or too hard, can be very helpful motivationally. It certainly is for me. (This could be analyzed in quite a lot of detail; maybe someone has.) Oh yeah: kids with ADHD have been shown to perform more poorly on the "Continuous Performance Task" (CPT) than controls. However, some researchers compared ADHD performance on the standard CPT with a modified CPT, which measured the same cognitive performance but with more game-like assessment software. The ADHD performance tended toward normal with the test in that format. So there's a motivational thing there, and having interesting/engaging feedback can make a big difference on performance. The study is at http://jad.sagepub.com/cgi/content/abstract/8/4/160 (DOI: 10.1177/1087054705278771)

...rejecting the raw empiricist impulse...
A major problem I have with the traditional definition of empiricism is that it doesn't encapsulate introspective observations. But such observations can be worked from, and are clearly related to actual physical phenomena (brain function, and in many cases to experience or conscious thought and action.) I find neuroimaging to be important, because it's capable of demonstrating, especially along with experimental methods, that our psyches really are more complex than many people think.

Unconscious mental process have actually been studied quite a bit by psychologists, and more recently by neuroscientists. This is done through experimental methods, and although what's done has limitations (for example, I've seen no study that conceives the unconscious in Jungian complexity, or even much in terms of Freudian defense mechanisms).

"Note here, that Marx attempted to show the ways in which the material conditions of people's lives, and not just what was inside their heads, produced some elements of their consciousness of reality, whether they were aware of it or not."
Yes, some. I argue that how people interpret and utilize their material conditions makes a great deal of difference in their consciousness of reality. Also, here's a plug for the relevance of non-material conditions for psychological development:
The conditions of learning and experience one has growing up, have a lot of influence, and material conditions are only one aspect. A child's experience interacting with others and observing adult behavior (including the manifestation of their mental content, i.e., language, expressions, and beliefs) makes a profound difference on development. For example, given two kids growing up in separate families with the same material conditions, one might grow up learning that things can be done to improve material conditions, like invention, better use of resources, construction, production, etc. Another might grow up with the idea that nothing can be done to improve conditions, that there is a fixed amount of material in the world, and that their family (and anyone else) was stuck with what it has, unless they take things from someone else. The latter is much more likely to join communism and run around robbing and murdering people who have things, without even considering there might be another way to improve their (or others') conditions. This is exactly what happened in China in the 20th century with effective genocide against the landlord class: some historical background (part of a whole series) is at http://english.epochtimes.com/news/4-12-13/24939.html (I highly suggest reading some of that to get a sense of what I think of when I hear "Marxism") and I also highly recommend the 3-disc documentary China - A Century of Revolution. (I'm especially in the mood to debate and tear apart ideas as the semester ends; which is a good example of your comment I quote next.)

How does causing them to sit, learn, and obey virtually assure that they will find a place in their minds to go and be free.
Good point, although one of the "places" is simply deactivation and slipping into semi-unconsciousness, especially for people with certain traits; and for others it is acting out. Also, if the forced structure enforces "attentional constraints" and is boring, it may not be feasible to fantasize during the period of situationally-induced boredom (i.e., an unengaging class.) At a minimum, it is generally considered unacceptable to draw or fantasize rather than paying attention.

Also, it induces cognitive dissonance that the actual "punishment" for failing to "obey" will be meted out within a few weeks -- and that rather than sinking into apathy or getting motivated to catch up, my energy is heading in abundant amounts toward doing mental tasks that seem worthwhile and meaningfully challenging to me. My GPA is going to be trash, and I'm actually kind of ambivalent about that. Maybe the best way to think of it is that my grades, and GPA, are just as much a representation of my evaluation of the class(es), they are a grade is of my performance. (On the assumption that my performance declines (or ceases) in response to whatever it is about the classes that induce exponentially increasing aversion.)

Plus even a 4.0 GPA wouldn't really measure abilities and knowledge that I see as most important (and we'll see in the coming years, in practice.)

I didn't address everything in the blog entry but I'd better...
[end hypomanic rant]